Orengedokuto and san’oshashinto improve memory deficits by inhibiting aging-dependent activation of glycogen synthase kinase-3b
Hironori Fujiwara a, *, Jun Yoshida a, Dya Fita Dibwe a, Suresh Awale a, Haruka Hoshino a, Hiroshi Kohama a, Hiroyuki Arai b, Yukitsuka Kudo b, Kinzo Matsumoto a
a b s t r a c t
Background and aim: The aging-dependent activation of glycogen synthase kinase-3b (GSK-3b) has been suggested to be important in the onset of dementia. To discover novel therapeutic Kampo medicines for dementia, we examined the effects of orengedokuto (OGT; 黃連解毒湯 hu´ang lia´n jieˇdú ta¯ng) and san’oshashinto (SST; 三黃瀉心湯 sa¯n hu´ang xie` x¯ın t¯ang) on memory deficits and GSK-3b activity in senescence-accelerated prone mice (SAMP8). Experimental procedure: The object recognition test (ORT) and conditioned fear memory test (CFT) were employed to elucidate short-term working memory and long-term fear memory. The activity of GSK-3b and the phosphorylation of related molecules were measured using a kinase assay and Western blotting. Results and conclusion: OGT and SST attenuated memory deficits in SAMP8 in ORT, but not in CFT. In ex vivo experiments, cortical GSK-3b activity was significantly stronger in SAMP8 than in SAMR1. The enhanced cortical GSK-3b activity in SAMP8 was accompanied by a significant increase in the level of phosphorylated collapsin response mediator protein-2 (CRMP2), an important factor that is involved in the regulation of microtubule stability. OGT and SST attenuated not only increases in cortical GSK-3b activity, but also the levels of phosphorylated CRMP2 in SAMP8. In vitro experiments, flavonoids con- tained in these kampo medicines, inhibited GSK-3b activity in concentration-dependent manners. These results suggest that OGT and SST prevent aging-induced short-term working memory deficits by inhibiting aging-dependent elevations in the cortical GSK-3b activity and subsequent CRMP2 phosphorylation.
1.Introduction
Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is one of the causative diseases of dementia and is accompanied by memory and cognition deficits in addition to the behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD). There are two pathological characteristics of AD: the deposition of insoluble senile plaques composed of amyloid-b protein1 and the intracellular accumulation of neurofibrillary tan- gles composed of tau-based paired helical filaments.2 Hyper- phosphorylated tau protein is a main component of neurofibril- lary tangles. Tau protein normally binds to microtubules, thereby contributing to cytoskeleton stabilization; however, once hyper- phosphorylated by several serine-threonine kinases such as glycogen synthase kinase-3b (GSK-3b), cyclin-dependent kinase-5, and mitogen-activated protein kinase,3,4 it dissociates from mi- crotubules. GSK-3b is involved in the onset of various diseases such as neuropathy, inflammatory disease, and cancer.5e8 GSK-3b is generally phosphorylated by other kinases to its inactive state, and is activated by the inhibition of phosphorylation, which occurs under pathological conditions.9 The activity of GSK-3b in the brain increases with aging,10 and the expression level and activity of GSK-
3b were found to be elevated in the brains of type II diabetic pa- tients with mild cognitive impairment.11 Based on these findings, we hypothesized that the regulation of GSK-3b activity represents an important strategy for effective therapy for dementia.
Collapsin response mediator protein-2 (CRMP2) is a member of the collapsin response mediator protein family. This protein is strongly expressed in the nervous system and is involved in kinesin-dependent axonal transport, neurite outgrowth and retraction, and microtubule dynamics.12,13 Similar to tau protein, CRMP2 is phosphorylated at the Thr509 and Thr514 residues by GSK-3b. This process reduces the binding affinity of CRMP2 for tubulin, resulting in the destabilization of microtubules.14,15 Syn- aptic deficits and the accumulation of phosphorylated CRMP2 have been reported in the brains of patients with dementia16,17 as well as in those of AD model animals.18 An increase in the phosphorylated form of CRMP2 as well as in synaptic dysfunction may occur in the early stage of AD. Recent clinical studies on AD patients showed that some Kampo medicines improved cognitive impairment, cerebral blood flow, and BPSD.19e22 Moreover, the ameliorative effects of Kampo med- icines have been demonstrated in various animal models of AD23e25 indicating that Kampo medicines are applicable as thera- peutic agents for AD. In the present study, we examined the effects of orengedokuto (OGT; 黃連解毒湯 hu´ang lia´n jieˇdú ta¯ng) and san’oshashinto (SST; 三黃瀉心湯 s¯an hua´ng xie` x¯ın t¯ang), Kampo formulae containing Scutellariae Radix (黄芩 hua´ng qín), on mem- ory deficits and the aging-dependent activation of GSK-3b in senescence-accelerated mouse-prone 8 (SAMP8), an animal model of aging and dementia. These formulae are often used to treat the symptoms of dementia in Japan26; however, the mechanisms un- derlying their effects have not yet been elucidated. The present results showed that OGT and SST prevented aging-induced cogni- tive impairments by suppressing aging-dependent enhancements in GSK-3b activity and CRMP2 phosphorylation. Our results provide a novel preventive strategy for dementia using Kampo medicines.
2.Materials and methods
2.1.Reagents
Baicalin, wogonin, and baicalein were obtained from Sigma- Aldrich (St. Louis, MO) and Wako Pure Chemical Industries, Ltd. (Osaka, Japan). All reagents and drugs used were of analytical grade.
2.2.Preparation and chemical profiling of OGT and SST
The following medicinal herbs, which conformed to Japanese Pharmacopeia XVI, were purchased from Tochimoto tenkai-do (Osaka, Japan) to prepare OGT and SST. OGT was extracted from a mixture of 6.0 parts Scutellariae Radix, 4.0 parts Coptidis Rhizoma (黃連 hua´ng li´an), 4.0 parts Gardeniae Fructus (山梔子 sh¯an zh¯ı zǐ), and 3.0 parts Phellodendri Cortex (黄柏 hua´ng bǎi). SST was extracted from a mixture of 6.0 parts Scutellariae Radix, 6.0 parts Coptidis Rhizoma, and 6.0 parts Rhei Rhizoma (大黄 da` hu´ang). The yields of the OGT and SST extracts were 7.71% and 7.95%, respec- tively. The chemical constituents of OGT and SST were analyzed using a Shimadzu LC-IT-TOF mass spectrometer equipped with an ESI interface. ESI parameters were as follows: sourcevoltage, +4.5 kV (positive ion mode) or —3.5 kV (negative ion mode); capillary temperature, 200 ◦C; and nebulizer gas, 1.5 l/min. A Waters Atlantis T3 column (2.1 mm × 100 mm) was maintainedat 40 ◦C. The mobile phase was a binary eluent of (A) 5 mMammonium acetate solution and (B) acetonitrile under the following gradient conditions: 0e30 min; linear gradient from 10% to 100% B, and 30e40 min; isocratic at 100% B. The flow rate was0.2 ml/min. Mass spectrometry data obtained from OGT and SST have been stored in the Wakan-Yaku DataBase system, Institute of Natural Medicine, University of Toyama (OGT: http://dentomed. toyama-wakan.net/en/information_on_experimental_kampo_ extracts/Orengedokuto-2016-KM/EXP004002; SST: http://dentomed. toyama-wakan.net/en/information_on_experimental_kampo_ extracts/Sanoushashinto-2016-KM/EXP016002). Voucher speci- mens had been deposited at our institute.
2.3.Animals
Five-week-old male SAMP8 and SAM-resistant (SAMR1) mice were obtained from Japan SLC (Shizuoka, Japan). The housing room was maintained at 24 ± 1 ◦C with 65% humidity and a 12-h light- dark cycle (lights on: 07:30-19:30). Food and water were given ad libitum. All animal research procedures used in the present study were in accordance with the Guiding Principles for the Care and Use of Animals (NIH Publications No. 80-23, revised in 1996). The present study was also approved by the Institutional Animal Use and Care Committee of the University of Toyama. All surgery was performed under anesthesia, and all efforts were made to minimize suffering. The present study was conducted according to the experimental schedule described in Supplementary Fig. S1.
2.4.Drug treatment
OGT (4250 mg/kg/day) or SST (4500 mg/kg/day) was adminis- tered intragastrically from the age of 6 months until all behavioral experiments were completed. In control groups, animals were treated with a water vehicle. These doses were approximately 30- fold higher than the typical daily doses for human therapy and in the ranges of herbal medicines that have been used in the study of animal models reported by our and other research groups.27,28
2.5.Behavioral analysis
2.5.1.Nobel object recognition test (ORT)
ORT was conducted as previously described with minor modifications.29 The apparatus consisted of a square arena (35 × 35 × 35 cm) with gray walls and a black floor. The objects for recognition had visual patterns or visually different shapes to be discriminated. ORT consisted of a sample trial and test trial. In the sample phase trial, each mouse was initially placed in the square arena with two identical objects, F1 and F2 (each of which was a 5.0 × 7.5 cm black cone), which were placed separately, and the mouse was allowed to freely explore the arena for five minutes. The total time that the mouse spent exploring each of the two objects was measured and the mouse was then returned to the home cage. In the test phase trial performed 30 min after the sample phase trial, one of the two objects was replaced by an identical copy (object F) and the other by a novel object (object N) and the total time that the mouse spent exploring each of the two objects was measured again. The performance of animals in these trials was video-recorded for later analyses and the exploration of an object was defined as directing the nose to the object at a distance of less than 2 cm according to previous studies. Mice that did not approach both objects during the 5-min observation period were excluded from the test trials. The time spent exploring each of the two ob- jects was analyzed with SMART® ver. 2.5 (PanLab, SLU, Spain) with a tri-wise module to detect the head, center of mass, and base of the tail. The box arena and objects were cleaned with 75% ethanol between trials to prevent a build-up of olfactory cues.
2.5.2.Conditioned fear memory test (CFT)
CFT was performed using the protocol of Ebihara et al. with minor modifications.30 Briefly, the equipment for fear conditioning consisted of a transparent acrylic chamber (30 cm × 30 cm × 30 cm) and a stainless-steel grid floor equipped with an electric shock generator/scrambler SGS-002®, CS Controller CSS-001®, and Cycle Timer CMT® (Muromachi Kikai. Co. Ltd., Tokyo, Japan). The equip-
ment was placed in a soundproof observation box (MC-050/CM, Muromachi Kikai, Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan) through which an auditory tone (Sonalert®, Mallory Sonalert Products Inc., Indianapolis, IN, USA) was delivered to the animal. In the training trial, animals were placed individually into the chamber and allowed to explore freely for 3 min. They then received an acoustic tone (2.9 kHz, 20 s, 80 dB) that co-terminated with electric foot shocks (0.8 mA, 2 s). The tone- foot shock pairing was repeated 5 times with 1-min intervals. One minute after the final foot shock delivery, mice were returned to their home cage.
Mice that did not respond to the foot shocks were excluded from the test trials. Contextual and auditory fear memories were examined 24 h and 6 days after the training trial, respectively. In the contextual memory test, mice were placed in the same chamber to provide contextual stimuli without an acoustic tone and allowed to move freely for 6 min. One minute after placing the an- imal in the chamber, freezing behavior during a 5-min period was recorded as an index of contextual-dependent fear memory. In measurements of auditory-dependent fear memory, mice were placed in another chamber for a total of 6 min. After a 3-min habituation period, the tone was delivered continuously for 3 min. Freezing behavior during the 3-min period was recorded as auditory-dependent fear memory. Animal behavior was video- recorded and analyzed automatically using the Smart® system. Freezing was defined as the absence of any movement, except for that related to respiration, and analyzed as a state with a movement speed no greater than 0.05 cm2/s.28,31
2.6.Preparation of lysates
The cortex and hippocampus were obtained from each mouse and homogenized in protein lysis buffer [RIPA buffer (Cell Signaling Technology Inc., U.S.A.), protein inhibitor cocktail (Roche Di- agnostics K. K., Japan), and 2 mM phenylmethylsulfonyl fluoride] using TissueLyser® (Qiagen, Osaka, Japan), and centrifuged at 1500×g at 4 ◦C for 10 min. The protein concentrations of superna- tants were measured using a BCA™ protein assay kit (Thermo Scientific, Rockford, IL, USA). Protein extracts containing 5 mg/ml
proteins were used in subsequent experiments.
2.7.Measurement of in vivo and ex vivo activities of GSK-3b
The activity of GSK-3b was measured using a kinase assay with two experimental paradigms: an in vitro analysis using recombi- nant active GSK-3b proteins (Sigma, St. Louis, MO, USA) and an ex vivo analysis using bio-samples extracted from animal brains after behavioral experiments. The kinase assay was conducted us- ing an ADP-Glo™ Kinase Assay Kit (Promega, WI, USA) according to the manufacturer’s instructions. Briefly, 5 ml of kinase reaction so- lution contained 16 mM TriseHCl (pH 7.5), 8 mM MgCl2, 0.04 mg/ ml bovine serum albumin, 0.4% DMSO, 2 mM b-mercaptoethanol, 0.2 mg/ml GSK3 substrate, 10 mM ATP, and 0.5 units of recombinant active GSK-3b proteins or 15 mg of protein extracts. The amount of protein was reduced to 1.5 mg in Lineweaver-Burk plot analysis. The kinase reaction solution was incubated at 25 ◦C for 1 h. Five mi- croliters of ADP-Glo™ Reagent was added to the reaction solution, which was then incubated at 25 ◦C for 1 h. Ten microliters of Kinase Detection Reagent was added to the reaction solution, and incu- bated at 25 ◦C for 1 h. Luminescence was measured on a microplate reader (FilterMax F5; Molecular Device, USA).
2.8.Western blotting
The phosphorylation of GSK-3b, tau, and CRMP2 was analyzed using Western blotting as previously described.28 Briefly, we randomly selected four mice from each group. Protein lysates from the hippocampus and cortex were obtained as described above. Protein extracts containing 10 mg of protein were applied to SDS- polyacrylamide gels and electrophoresed. Separated proteins were transferred to a polyvinylidene difluoride membrane (Immuno-Blot® membrane, Bio-Rad Laboratories, Hercules, CA, USA). Blots were blocked with 2.5% bovine serum albumin in 0.1% Tween 20 containing TBS-T, and probed with the primary anti- bodies described below using a 1:1000 dilution at 4 ◦C overnight,
followed by an incubation with an anti-rabbit or anti-mouse IgG antibody HRP-linked in a dilution of 1:2000 at room temperature for 2 h. After washing in TBS-T, chemiluminescence was detected using ImmobilonTM Western Chemiluminescent HRP Substrate (Millipore, Billerica, MA, USA). Immunoreactive bands were visu- alized and analyzed with ImageQuant LAS-4000 and ImageQuant TL® (GE Healthcare Japan, Tokyo, Japan).
The primary antibodies used were: an anti-GSK-3b rabbit monoclonal antibody, anti-phospho-GSK-3b (Ser9) rabbit mono- clonal antibody, anti-phospho-tau (Ser396) rabbit monoclonal antibody, anti-phospho-CRMP2 (Thr514) rabbit monoclonal anti- body, anti-CRMP2 rabbit monoclonal antibody, and anti-GAPDH mouse monoclonal antibody.
2.9.Statistical analysis
Data are expressed as the mean ± S.E.M. Data obtained in ORT were analyzed by the paired t-test for two groups according to a previous study. Data obtained in the fear conditioning test and neurochemical studies were analyzed by the Student’s t-test for two groups or a one-way ANOVA followed by the Stu- denteNewmaneKeuls test for multiple comparisons. P values of <0.05 were considered to be significant. The analysis was con- ducted using SigmaPlot® ver 12.0 software (SYSTAT Software Inc., Richmond, CA, USA).
3.Results
3.1.OGT and SST ameliorated the non-spatial cognitive deficits of SAMP8
First, the object recognition test (ORT) was performed to assess the effects of OGT and SST on non-spatial cognitive deficits in SAMP8. Sample trials of ORT revealed no significant differences in the total time spent exploring two identical objects between each group (SAMR1: t ¼ —0.485, df ¼ 10, p ¼ 0.638; SAMP8-vehicle: t ¼ 1.352, df ¼ 9, p ¼ 0.209; SAMP8-OGT: t ¼ 0.524, df ¼ 10, p ¼ 0.612; SAMP8-SST: t ¼ 1.274, df ¼ 9, p ¼ 0.235) (Fig. 1A). In the test trials, SAMR1 spent a significantly longer time exploring a novel object than a familiar object (t ¼ 2.357, df ¼ 10, P < 0.05), indicating a preference for novelty. In contrast, SAMP8 showed no preference for the novel object (SAMP8-vehicle: t ¼ 2.222, df ¼ 9, P ¼ 0.053). The treatment of SAMP8 with OGT or SST normalized novel object recognition behavior in this group, the animals in which spent a significantly longer time on the novel object than on the familiar object (SAMP8-OGT: t ¼ 4.01, df ¼ 10, P < 0.01; SAMP8- SST: t ¼ 7.715, df ¼ 9, P < 0.01) (Fig. 1B).
3.2.OGT and SST had no effects on the fear memory deficit in SAMP8
Next, we performed the conditioned fear memory test (CFT) in Fig. 1. Effects of OGT and SST on object recognition performance in SAMP8. The sample (A) and test (B) trials of the object recognition test (ORT) were conducted as described in the text. The numbers in each group were 11 (SAMR1-water), 10 (SAMP8-water), 11 (SAMP8-OGT), and 11 (SAMP8-SST). Each data point represents the mean ± S.E.M. *P < 0.05,
**P < 0.01 vs. the time spent exploring F1 (paired t-test) order to assess the effects of OGT and SST on long-term fear memory deficits in SAMP8 (Fig. 2). The freezing times of SAMP8 in the contextual (Fig. 2A) and auditory (Fig. 2B) memory tests were significantly less than those of SAMR1 (contextual: t ¼ 3.142, df ¼ 19, p < 0.05; auditory: t ¼ 2.443, df ¼ 19, p < 0.05). However, OGT and SST failed to improve context- and auditory-dependent fear memories (contextual: F(2,28) ¼ 1.002, p ¼ 0.380; auditory: F(2,28) ¼ 2.313, p ¼ 0.118).
3.3.OGT and SST inhibited age-dependent GSK-3b activation in SAMP8 cortex without phosphorylated regulation
After completing the behavioral experiments, we analyzed the effects of OGT and SST on the ex vivo activities of GSK-3b in the brains of these animals. The activity of GSK-3b in the cortex was approximately 25% higher in SAMP8 than in age-matched SAMR1 (t ¼ —3.570, df ¼ 6, p < 0.05) (Fig. 3A1). Moreover, the elevation in GSK-3b activity was abolished in SAMP8 treated with OGT and SST [F(2,9) ¼ 8.281, p < 0.05, SAMP8-vehicle vs. SAMP8-OGT: p < 0.05; SAMP8-vehicle vs. SAMP8-SST: p < 0.05] (Fig. 3A1). In contrast, no significant difference was observed in the hippocampal activity of GSK-3b between each group [SAMR1 vs. SAMP8: t ¼ 0.992, df ¼ 6, p ¼ 0.359; among treatment: F(2,9) ¼ 0.0126, p ¼ 0.988] (Fig. 3A2). A significant difference was not noted in GSK-3b activity in the cortex when activity was analyzed at the age of 2 months (t ¼ 1.363, df ¼ 6, p ¼ 0.222) (Fig. 3B). Next, the enzyme kinetics was analyzed
Fig. 2. Effects of OGT and SST on long-term fear memory deficits in SAMP8. The freezing times of animals under the contextual (A) and auditory (B) experimental conditions were measured as described in the text. OGT and SST were administered orally at doses of 4250 and 4500 mg/kg/day for 5 weeks before the test. The numbers of each group were 11 (SAMR1-water), 10 (SAMP8-water), 11 (SAMP8-OGT), and 10 (SAMP8-SST). Each data column represents the mean ± S.E.M. *P < 0.05 vs. SAMR1 group (Student's t-test). #p < 0.05 significantly different from the water-administered SAMP8 group (Student-Newman-Keuls test).
Fig. 3. Effects of OGT and SST administration on the ex vivo activity of GSK-3b in the cortex and hippocampus of SAMP8. GSK-3b activity was measured in cortical and hippocampal homogenate preparations obtained from SAMP8 and SAMR1 at the age of 8 (A1, 2) or 2 months (B). Each data column represents the mean ± S.E.M. (n ¼ 4). *P < 0.05 significantly different from SAMR1 (Student's t-test). #P < 0.05 significantly different from the SAMP8-vehicle group (Student-Newman-Keuls test). (C) Lineweaver-Burk plot of the GSK-3b activity between SAMR1 and SAMP8 (C1) and GSK-3b inhibition by OGT and SST in SAMP8 (C2). V represents the rate of enzyme reaction (mM/h). Each data point represents the mean ± S.E.M. (n ¼ 3). with Lineweaver-Burk plot to determine the type of the age- dependent increase of GSK-3b activity in SAMP8 or the inhibitory types by OGT and SST (Fig. 3C). In the GSK-3b in the cortex of SAMP8, Vmax increased from 3.015 to 3.558 but Km decreased from0.098 to 0.089, as compared with that of SAMR1 (Fig. 3C1). More- over, OGT and SST showed mixed inhibition such that Vmax decreased to 3.306 and 3.018 and Km increased to 0.202 and 0.171, respectively (Fig. 3C2).
The expression levels of non-phosphorylated and Ser9- phosphorylated GSK-3b in the cortex of SAMR1 and vehicle- and test drug-treated SAMP8 were measured by Western blotting (Fig. 4). No significant differences were found in the expression Fig. 4. Effects of OGT and SST on expression levels of GSK-3b and phosphorylated GSK3b (P-GSK-3b) in the cortex of SAMP8. A) Typical photos indicating the levels of P- GSK-3b, GSK-3b, and GAPDH in the cortex obtained from SAMR1 and SAMP8 treated with water, OGT, and SST. Columns in each group represent biological replicates. B) Quantitative comparisons of the expression levels of P-GSK-3b, GSK-3b, and GAPDH among different groups. Each data column represents the mean ± S.E.M. obtained from4 brain samples. *P < 0.05 significantly different from SAMR1 (Student's t-test). #P < 0.05 significantly different from the SAMP8-vehicle group (Student-Newman- Keuls test).levels of the P-GSK-3b or GSK-3b between SAMR1 and vehicle- treated SAMP8 (P-GSK-3b: t ¼ 0.598, df ¼ 6, p ¼ 0.572; GSK-3b: t ¼ 2.421, df ¼ 6, p ¼ 0.052). Moreover, the administration of OGT and SST had no effects on the expression levels of GSK-3b or P-GSK- 3b in the cortex of SAMP8 [P-GSK-3b: F(2,9) ¼ 2.151, P ¼ 0.172; GSK-3b: F(2,9) ¼ 0.838, P ¼ 0.464) (Fig. 4).
3.4.OGT and SST inhibited the phosphorylation of CRMP2 but not tau in the cortex of SAMP8
We confirmed whether aging-dependent elevations in cortical GSK-3b activity in SAMP8 affects the phosphorylation of the downstream molecules, tau and CRMP2, and, if so, how the administration of OGT and SST changes the levels of the phos- phorylated forms of tau and CRMP2 in the cortex of SAMP8. Tau Ser396 phosphorylation in the cortex showed no significant differences between each group [SAMR1 v.s. SAMP8: t ¼ 0.511, df ¼ 6, p ¼ 0.627; among treatments: F(2,9) ¼ 0.207, p ¼ 0.816] (Fig. 5A and B1). P-CRMP2 levels in the cortex were significantly higher in vehicle-treated SAMP8 than in SAMR1 (t ¼ —6.706, df ¼ 6, p < 0.01), and the administration of OGT and SST significantly decreased the elevated cortical level of P-CRMP2 in SAMP8 [F(2,9) ¼ 7.126, p ¼ 0.014, SAMP8-vehicle vs.SAMP8-OGT: p ¼ 0.014; SAMP8- vehicle vs.SAMP8-SST: p ¼ 0.022] without affecting the expres- sion of CRMP2 [SAMR1 v.s. SAMP8: t ¼ 1.480, df ¼ 6, p ¼ 0.189; among treatments: F(2,9) ¼ 0.817, p ¼ 0.472] (Fig. 5B2 and 3).
3.5.Flavonoid compounds contained in Scutellariae Radix inhibited GSK-3b activity
Finally, we examined the inhibitory effects of GSK-3b activity by active ingredients contained in OGT and SST. Among them, three flavonoid compounds, baicalin, wogonin, and baicalein, contained in S. Radix inhibited the in vitro activity of GSK-3b in a
concentration-dependent manner [baicalin: F(3, 8) ¼ 29.864, P < 0.01; wogonin: F(4, 10) ¼ 11.445, P < 0.001; baicalein: F(4, 10) ¼ 5.129, P ¼ 0.016] (Fig. 6 and Supplementary Fig. S2).
4.Discussion
The present study using ORT revealed that SAMP8 exhibited impaired object recognition performance, an index of short-term non-spatial working memory, in a manner that was reversed by Fig. 5. Effects of OGT and SST on cortical levels of phospho-Tau (P-Tau) and phosphor- CRMP2 (P-CRMP2) in the cortex of SAMP8. A) Typical photos indicating the levels of P- Tau, P-CRMP2, and total CRMP2 in the cortex obtained from SAMR1 and SAMP8 treated with water, OGT, and SST. Columns in each group represent biological replicates. B) Quantitative comparisons of the levels of P-tau (B1), P-GSK-3b (B2), and total CRMP2 (B3). Each data column represents the mean ± S.E.M. obtained from 4 brain samples. *P < 0.05 significantly different from SAMR1 (Student's t-test). #P < 0.05 significantly different from the SAMP8-vehicle group (Student-Newman-Keuls test).
Fig. 6. Effects of baicalin, wogonin, and baicalein, flavonoids contained in OGT and SST, on the activity of recombinant GSK-3b. The activities of recombinant GSK-3b were measured using an ADP-Glo™ Kinase Assay Kit. Each data point represents the mean ± S.E.M. (n ¼ 3). *P < 0.05, *P < 0.01 significantly different from the vehicle control (Student-Newman-Keuls test) the administration of OGT and SST. Moreover, the cognitive deficit in SAMP8 was accompanied by significant elevations in cortical GSK-3b activity. Since no significant differences were observed in GSK-3b activity in SAMP8 in 2-month-old SAMP8 or in hippo- campal tissues in SAMP8, our results provide at least two important suggestions. The increase in cortical GSK-3b activity in SAMP8 may have occurred in an aging-dependent manner and contributed to the development of non-spatial short-term working memory def- icits in this animal model. Furthermore, we may infer that the administration of OGT and SST suppresses aging-dependent increases in cortical GSK-3b activity, thereby preventing aging- induced non-spatial cognitive deficits.
Non-spatial memory detected in ORT is known to be related to short-term episodic memory in humans,32 which is impaired in the early stages of dementia.33 The entorhinal area is essential for short-term episodic memory to function normally.34 Moreover, evidence indicates that neurofibrillary tangles are formed in the entorhinal cortex in the early stages of AD, and spread to other parts as the disease progresses.35 Collectively, the present results allow us to infer that the elevated activity of GSK-3b impairs the function of the entorhinal area adjacent to the cortex in the early stages of AD, thereby contributing to the progression of the disease, and also that OGT and SST may be able to suppress early AD progression by inhibiting GSK-3b in the cortex or entorhinal area. In contrast to short-term working memory deficits in ORT, long- term memory deficits in SAMP8 in CFT were not susceptible to the treatment with OGT and SST. Furthermore, GSK-3b activity in the hippocampus was similar among the animal groups regardless of whether they were treated with OGT and/or SST. These results indicate that the contribution of hippocampal GSK-3b activity to long-term fear memory deficits is negligible under our experi- mental conditions.
Although the mechanisms underlying aging-dependent changes in GSK-3b activity currently remain unclear, it is suggested that the complicated factors are involved, because that Lineweaver-Burk plot revealed that GSK-3b of SAMP8 has higher both of a maximum rate of the enzyme reaction and an affinity for GSK-3b substrate than that of SAMR1. For example, the aging-induced activation of GSK-3b in the cortex may be attributable to func- tional changes in endogenous molecules with regulatory roles in GSK-3b activity. Evidence indicates that GSK-3b forms a complex with other endogenous molecules such as Axin, adenomatous polyposis coli, casein kinase a, and b-catenin and may be regulated by a Wnt signaling cascade.36 Ye et al.37 previously reported that GSK-3b was activated by attenuating the Wnt2 signal, which ac- companies cellular senescence. Furthermore, the down-regulation of an inactivation system for GSK-3b activity may be caused by aging, which apparently increases the activity of this enzyme in the cortex. Other kinases such as Akt reportedly convert GSK-3b to an inactive form by phosphorylating the Ser9 residue of the GSK-3b protein.10 However, this does not appear to be likely because no significant differences were observed in P-GSK-3b levels in the cortex between SAMP8 and SAMR1. Further investigations are needed in order to elucidate the exact mechanisms by which aging activates the cortical activity of GSK-3b.
Tau and CRMP2 are substrates for the activity of GSK-3b and are involved in stabilizing the microtubules of nerve cells.3,15 Further- more, the phosphorylation of these proteins caused by the activity of GSK-3b is known to be closely related to dysfunctional neuronal transmission and cognitive deficits in patients with dementia.10,11 The present results revealed that the cortical levels of P-CRMP2, but not P-tau were significantly higher in 8-month-old SAMP8 than in age-matched SAMR1. These results not only indicate that the aging-induced activation of GSK-3b in the cortex links more pref- erentially to the phosphorylation of CRMP2 than tau protein, but also suggest that the link between GSK-3b activation and CRMP2 phosphorylation mainly contributes to memory deficits in a model of aging. Moreover, since the increase in cortical P-CRMP2 levels was attenuated in SAMP8 treated with OGT and SST, the adminis- tration of OGT and SST appears to prevent aging-induced working memory deficits by suppressing GSK-3b activation and/or the link between GSK activation and GSK-3b-mediated CRMP2 phos- phorylation in the cortex. This appears to be supported by studies using other natural products. Wang et al. reported that curcumin, a bioactive component of Curcuma longa, ameliorated b-amyloid-induced cognitive dysfunction by suppressing the hyper-phosphorylation of CRMP2.18 Furthermore, in CRMP2 phosphorylation-deficient knock-in mice, b-amyloid failed to induce memory deficits in the ORT.38
Several researchers have demonstrated that baicalin, wogonin, and baicalein, which are flavonoid constituents of S. Radix con- tained in OGT and SST, exert anti-dementia effects in several animal models of AD39e41 Collectively, our in vitro results suggest that these flavonoids contribute, at least in part, to the suppressive ef- fects of OGT and SST on aging-induced GSK-3b activation as well as on aging-induced working memory deficits in mice. However, since the inhibition of in vitro GSK-3b activity by baicalin, wogonin, and baicalein was very limited, we cannot exclude the possibility that OGT- and SST-derived chemical constituents other than these fla- vonoids also contribute to the effects of OGT and SST observed in the present study. The fact that OGT and SST revealed the mixed inhibition in the Lineweaver-Burk plot also suggests that multiple compounds are involved in the inhibition of GSK-3b. This possi- bility is currently being investigated in our laboratory.
5.Conclusion
The present study demonstrated that the administration of OGT and SST ameliorated aging-induced episodic memory deficits, possibly by inhibiting the aging-dependent activation of GSK-3b as well as subsequent CRMP2 phosphorylation. Thus, these Kampo formulae may offer a new preventive/therapeutic strategy from the early stages of dementia.
Conflicts of interest
The authors and contributors of this work declare no conflicts of interest.
Acknowledgments
This study was in part supported by a Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science to HF (JP25860075), a 2012 Grant-in-Aid from Mochida Memorial Foun- dation for CBD3063 Medical and Pharmaceutical Research (HF), a 2012 Grant- in-Aid from Tamura Science and Technology Foundation (HF), and 2017 Director Leadership Expenses, University of Toyama (HF).